The Effect of Training in Verbal Self-Guidance on
Performance Effectiveness in a MBA Program
TRAVOR C. BROWN,
GARY P. LATHAM,
Abstract
The present field experiment examined the effects of
training in verbal self-guidance (VSG) and a motivational
intervention, goal setting, on the performance effectiveness
of students in a MBA program. Performance effectiveness
was assessed in terms of outcome (i.e., GPA) and
behavioural (i.e., interpersonal skills) measures as well as
a composite criterion (performance effectiveness). MBA
students (n = 126) were randomly assigned to a 2 (VSG
training/control) x 2 (goal setting/do your best) factorial
design. The results revealed a main effect for VSG on performance
effectiveness. There was an interaction effect
such that participants who were trained in VSG and set
goals had the highest level of performance effectiveness.
A subsequent analysis revealed that the main effect for
VSG on performance was spurious due to an ordinal
interaction effect. Hence, VSG may be most effective
when combined with goal setting.
Verbal self-guidance (
VSG) involves people verbalizing
their thought processes as they consider a problem,
discover information relevant to this problem,
generate potential solutions, consider the advantages
and disadvantages of each alternative solution, and
implement what is considered to be the best solution
(Brown, 2003). Thus,
VSG refers to self-talk, but one’s
self-discourse can vary widely, depending on what
aspect of one’s functioning one seeks to influence.
Meichenbaum (1971, 1975, 1977), a clinical psychologist,
developed a
VSG training method, which
he labeled functional self-talk. This training teaches
people to systematically talk themselves through
ways to overcome obstacles to performing effectively.
In essence, this technique involves training people to
change their negative, dysfunctional self-statements
(e.g., I can’t solve this problem) to positive, functional
self-statements (e.g., I can break this problem into
parts and solve one part at a time). The training program
consists of a three-step process whereby a participant:
1) observes a clinician model effective selfstatements
that guide the person to the actions needed
to take to master a task, 2) performs a task while
verbally instructing oneself, and 3) performs a task
while verbally instructing oneself covertly.
Research Concerning
VSG
Much of the research concerning
VSG is limited to
studies of children in clinical, counseling or educational
settings. In these settings,
VSG has been shown
to have a positive effect on task mastery (i.e., ability).
For example, training in
VSG reduces errors made by
impulsive children (Meichenbaum & Goodman,
1971), increases reading and listening comprehension
skills of children (Schunk & Rice, 1984, 1985), and
improves the creativity of students (Meichenbaum,
1975).
The purpose of the present study was to examine
the external validity of
VSG training for adults performing
organizationally relevant tasks, namely, peo-
ple in a
MBA program. A potential boundary condition
for
VSG may be the age of the participants.
Previous studies showing the effectiveness of
VSG
have primarily involved children. Children may be
more amenable to learning to focus on and change
their self-talk than are adults. As William James
noted more than a century ago, habits are formed
early in life. By adulthood, they are “set like plaster”
(James, 1892, p. 375).
A second boundary variable for the effectiveness
of
VSG may be the setting. Adults in an MBA program
may be less willing to acquire the skills necessary for
changing their dysfunctional self-talk than are clients
in clinical/counseling settings. As Halpern (2004)
noted, an adult’s motivation to learn a new skill
depends in part on the characteristics of the environment.
A clinical/counseling setting may be more
amenable for teaching
VSG skills than an organizational
setting as the former allows the person to conceal
difficulties mastering a new task, and will not
threaten a person’s status among group members. A
pilot test of
VSG training in a previous year’s MBA
class revealed that many people derisively labeled
the process as “psycho-babble.” Nevertheless, the
potential theoretical significance of
VSG is that it may
be a self-regulation process that explains in part a
person’s performance effectiveness in an organizational
setting. As Vygotsky (1962) noted, inner speech
is the primary vehicle for thought and self-direction.
The practical significance of a positive finding is that
VSG
is an inexpensive training technique. As such,
VSG
may prove to be a relatively straightforward,
self-administered intervention.
To date, there have been very few
VSG training
studies involving adults in organizational settings.
Moreover, in two of those studies
VSG was imbedded
within other training interventions. Neck and Manz
(1996) used the “thought self-leadership technique”
(
TSL) that included VSG and mental imagery. The
training had no effect on job performance.
Waung (1995) found no significant effects for
VSG
in her study of two orientation-training programs. In
addition to
VSG, the treatment package included cognitive
restructuring and a realistic job preview. The
comparison group received a realistic job preview
that included inculcating coping behaviours. Four
weeks post-training, contrary to Waung’s hypotheses,
there were no significant differences between the
two conditions in terms of anxiety, intention to
remain in, and commitment to, the organization.
Moreover,
VSG participants experienced higher voluntary
turnover than those in the comparison group.
One potential explanation for these findings, offered
by Waung, is that
VSG participants reported that they
had received more negative information than did the
participants in the comparison group.
A second explanation, not reported by Waung
(1995), was the short duration of the training program,
namely a maximum of 35 minutes. This time
period may have increased participants’ awareness of
their negative self-statements without allowing for
the acquisition of requisite skills for changing these
statements to positive self-guidance. Third, the comparison
group was not a control group per se as these
participants were given training on coping behaviours.
This training may have bolstered the dependent
variables.
Using
VSG only, Millman and Latham (2001) found
that seven, two-hour training sessions, conducted
over a two and a half week period, resulted in a significantly
greater number of displaced managers
finding jobs within nine months of training, relative
to participants in the control group. Two limitations
of the study included the use of a quasi-experimental
design, as not all participants were randomly
assigned to conditions, and the small sample size.
Brown (2003) also used a quasi-experimental
design to examine the effect of
VSG training on team
performance on simulated tasks. Performance
increased significantly relative to a control group. As
the unit of analysis was the team, the effect of
VSG on
an individual’s performance was not examined.
Based on the fact that two of these four studies
found that training in
VSG affects performance positively
in organizational contexts, we hypothesized
that
VSG should improve the performance of adults
in a MBA program.
Goal Setting
None of the previously cited
VSG studies in clinical/
counseling or industrial organizational psychology
examined the explicit effects of goals on performance.
Yet, goal setting is implicitly imbedded in
VSG
. VSG requires an individual to turn specific negative
self-statements (e.g., I can’t find a job) to positive
self-statements (e.g., I can update my resume) in
order to attain a specific end goal (e.g., employment).
In the present study, goal setting was made explicit in
order to determine if it improves the effectiveness of
VSG
. However, because of theory and empirical
research, we did not hypothesize a main effect for
goal setting. As a theory of motivation, goal setting
assumes that: 1) the person has the requisite knowledge
and ability to perform the task, and 2) goal setting
activates this task knowledge (Locke, 2000;
Locke & Latham, 1990). Therefore, a difficult goal
cues the individual’s attention to choose to exert
effort and persistence to attain it, resulting in a main
Verbal Self-Guidance 3
effect for goals on performance. When a task is complex
and the person lacks the requisite task knowledge
or skill, setting a specific, difficult performance
goal can have a deleterious effect on performance.
For example, Kanfer and Ackerman (1989), in a field
study of Air Force trainees who were mastering a
flight simulator, found that during the declarative
stage of learning, participants need to focus their
attention on understanding ways to perform the task.
Hence, setting a specific, difficult goal decreased performance
as it diverted participants’ attention away
from discovering and encoding the necessary strategies
to perform the task. They concluded that goal
setting is effective only after individuals encode and
store the necessary task rules, as well as integrate the
sequencing of motor and cognitive processes for performing
the task effectively. These findings have been
replicated by others (Seijts & Latham, 2001; Winters
& Latham, 1996).
The assumption underlying the present study is
that first year
MBAs enter a new environment where
they have yet to develop the necessary behavioural
skills to work effectively in a team setting. The
MBA
program differs from a traditional undergraduate
program in at least three ways. First, unlike most
undergraduate programs, some classes are only several
weeks long while others run a full semester.
Second, the people with whom they regularly interact
have widely different academic backgrounds
(e.g., engineering, law, liberal arts, physical sciences,
education, health sciences, nursing, etc.). Many
MBA
students do not have an undergraduate degree in
business. Third, and most importantly, the
MBA program
requires extensive teamwork. Group assignments
are an integral part of most, if not all, courses.
People are permanently assigned to a team for at
least an entire semester, and sometimes for a full academic
year. Furthermore, there is fierce competition
among people for grades as many organizational
recruiters use grade point average (
GPA) as a criterion
for selection. The business school puts great pressure
on
MBA students to secure jobs with prestigious firms
as starting salaries and place of employment can
impact both the prestige and the ranking of a business
school in the popular press. Adding to the
expectations of, and stress placed on
MBA students, is
the emphasis by recruiters on evidence of interpersonal
(i.e., teamwork) skills. Thus adapting to the
environment of the
MBA program requires the acquisition
of interpersonal skills, a high GPA, and the
motivation to do so.
Goal Setting and
VSG
Based on the extant research, we hypothesized
that
VSG, with its emphasis on the discovery and
affirmation of one’s ability (e.g., “Ah, all I have to do
to succeed is…”), increases a person’s performance.
Based on Locke’s (2000) conclusions, we hypothesized
an interaction effect such that participants who
receive training in
VSG and set a specific high goal
have the highest level of performance effectiveness.
Locke argued that on a complex task there is typically
an interaction effect between task knowledge and
goals. Specifically, he argued that this interaction
occurs when: 1) the goal alone cannot activate existing
task knowledge because the person lacks such
knowledge and 2) this task knowledge is derived
from other sources (e.g., peers, professors), which
when combined with goals, results in the highest
level of performance in the high goal, high task
knowledge condition. Thus, we hypothesized that
the motivational effects of goal setting are beneficial
in this study only when training in
VSG enables an
individual to develop and affirm behaviours to overcome
obstacles to his/her performance.
VSG facilitates
the discovery of task behaviours because it
focuses attention on discovering and encoding the
necessary strategies to perform the task at hand.
Performance Criteria
There has been a long debate in the literature concerning
the choice of an appropriate performance criterion
(see Latham & Wexley, 1994). The performance
criteria in the present study were dictated by the setting.
MBA
programs have been criticized by both the
media (Jack, 2001), as well as the Association to
Advance Collegiate Schools of Business, which is the
accreditation body for
MBA programs in North
America (Olian et al., 2002), for developing people
who graduate with strong analytical yet weak interpersonal
skills. Business leaders and educators have
demanded that this situation be corrected (Mason,
1992; Olian et al., 2002; Pfeffer, 1998).
The MBA director in the business school, where
this study took place, assigns
MBA students to teams.
Both MBA students from previous years, as well as
faculty, had observed that poor interpersonal skills
had an adverse effect on students’ academic performance
in the program, namely
GPA. Sue-Chan and
Latham (2004) found that
MBA students lack the
knowledge and skills necessary to improve themselves
as team-players. Consequently, both an outcome
and a behavioural measure of performance
effectiveness were used to evaluate the effect of the
VSG
training program on the person’s performance.
The outcome measure in the present study was the
student’s
GPA. As noted earlier, many organizational
recruiters use this measure as a basis for hiring grad-
4 Brown and Latham
uating
MBA students, and faculty use GPA as an indicator
of a student’s mastery of course material.
Hence, this measure is highly relevant to a
MBA student.
A limitation of an outcome measure is that it is
often influenced by factors beyond the individual’s
control (e.g., quality of teaching; number of exams
scheduled in a given day; idiosyncratic grading bias
of instructors). Thus, a behavioural measure of a person’s
effectiveness was also used in this study, namely,
the person’s interpersonal skills. Interpersonal
skills are defined as the ability to communicate, work
collaboratively with others, manage time, empower/
delegate, as well as motivate/persuade self and
others (de Janasz, Dowd, & Schneider, 2002; Whetten
& Cameron, 2002).
A limitation of behavioural criteria is their susceptibility
to errors in observation (e.g., leniency, halo,
contrast effects). Multiple operationalization of performance
effectiveness in terms of an outcome and
behavioural measure decreases the likelihood of a
Type II error. In
MBA programs in general, and in this
MBA
program in particular, interpersonal skills and
GPA
are interrelated. This is because a student’s GPA
is affected by how well individuals perform as team
members in their respective teams. Team assignments
influence 40-80% of the person’s course grade
in each course of the
MBA program. Previous
research has shown that behavioural performance
measures, developed through a systematic job analysis,
correlate with performance outcome measures
(Latham & Wexley, 1977; Taggar & Brown, 2001).
Thus a composite criterion was also used in this
study as this measure is the norm for making administrative
decisions (e.g., hiring, promoting) in organizational
settings (Schmidt & Kaplan, 1971).
In summary, the present study differs from the
four earlier
VSG studies in industrial-organizational
psychology (Brown, 2003; Millman & Latham, 2001;
Neck & Manz, 1996; Waung, 1995) in that it used an
experimental rather than a quasi-experimental
design, where there was no confounding of treatments,
and where the consequences for the individual
were “real.” People were randomly assigned to
conditions. The effect of goal setting was examined
explicitly rather than implicitly. The outcomes for the
person included an appraisal of interpersonal skills
by peers as well as grades assigned by faculty members.
The evaluation of the training program included
both behavioral and outcome performance measures
as well as a composite criterion. Thus the probability
of making a Type II error regarding the effect
of the intervention in bringing about a change in the
person’s effectiveness was minimized. Two hypotheses
were tested.
Hypothesis 1: People trained in VSG have significantly
higher performance than those who are not trained in
VSG.
Hypothesis 2: There is an interaction effect between VSG
and goal setting on performance.
Method
Sample
Adults enrolled in the first year
MBA class (n =
126) of a Canadian university were randomly
assigned to a 2 (
VSG, control) x 2 (goal, do your best)
factorial design. This number represented 95.46% of
the students (n = 132). On average, these MBAs were
29 years old, male (59.50%) with 4 1/2 years of fulltime
employment experience. Participants did not
differ significantly from nonparticipants on sex, age,
or work experience.
Procedure
The procedure used in this experiment consisted
of the following steps: 1) development of a behavioural
measure of interpersonal skills, 2) random
assignment to experimental conditions, 3) goal setting
intervention, 4) training in
VSG, and 5) data collection.
Interpersonal skills. Interpersonal skills (see Table 1)
were assessed using behavioural observation scales
(
BOS; Latham & Wexley, 1977, 1994) developed by
Sue-Chan and Latham (2004). Using a previous firstyear
MBA
class, a job analysis, namely, the critical
incident technique (Flanagan, 1954), was conducted
with faculty who teach in the
MBA program as well
as
MBA students. This job analysis asked people to
recall examples of both effective and ineffective teamwork
or interpersonal behaviours that led to high or
low
GPA in the program. These behaviours were used
to develop
BOS. BOS were used because they are reliable
and valid, and they correlate positively with performance
outcome measures (e.g., Latham, 1997;
Latham & Wexley, 1977; Taggar & Brown, 2001).
Assignment to teams. Each year the director of the
MBA
program assigns each student to a team. Steps
are taken to ensure that the teams are relatively balanced
in terms of sex, quantitative/qualitative
undergraduate degrees, and international/national
students. Group size in the present study ranged
from five to six individuals. The tasks performed by
these teams were similar to those performed by
teams in other organizational settings. Specifically,
these tasks included multidisciplinary analyses and
presentations, as well as consulting projects based on
Verbal Self-Guidance 5
organizational site visits. As consulting groups, the
teams met client organizations, analyzed data, made
recommendations, and presented their findings to
key stakeholders. These team projects, as noted earlier,
represented a minimum of 40% of the final grade
in each MBA course.
The roles of team members were highly interdependent.
Each team was identifiable by a name (they
also had web folders, mailboxes, etc.) and each had
total authority on project planning regarding assignment
of tasks to individuals among the team. Each
team also made decisions that had important consequences
for them, both as a team and as individuals
within the team, in terms of
GPA. The only directions
given to the teams by faculty members were the task
assignments and their completion dates.
Experimental conditions. A 2 (
VSG/control) by 2
(goal setting/no goal setting) factorial design was
used in this experiment. All participants were shown
the
BOS and informed that the interpersonal skills
contained on them were important for performing
well in the
MBA program. Given the use of teams in
the business school, behaviours on the
BOS were
labeled teamwork skills. Immediately following the
presentation of the BOS, participants were invited to
participate in the study. Specifically, they were
informed that: a) the purpose of the study was to
assess the effectiveness of different approaches to
performing effectively in the MBA program; b) it was
important not to share training program details with
other people as it could cause contamination; and c)
we randomly assigned participants to conditions in
order to control for extraneous variables. This was
done without regard to a person’s team as the unit of
analysis in this study was the individual. At no time
did we share our hypotheses with participants or faculty
members.
Goal setting. Participants (n = 62) in the goal-setting
condition met in a large room for approximately
30 minutes. There they were explicitly asked to set a
specific, difficult goal for the
BOS score that they
would work to attain as individuals. They then met
in small groups of four to six people to discuss their
rationale for their goal because discussing one’s goal
can increase goal commitment (Locke & Latham,
1990). At the end of the discussion, they were
informed that they could modify their goals based on
this conversation. Goal difficulty and goal commitment
were assessed at this time. We did not require a
goal for
GPA as all of them wanted a “perfect” 4.0.
Participants in the no goal setting, do your best
(
DYB) condition (n = 64) met in a separate room.
They, too, received the
BOS. Rather than setting a specific,
difficult goal, they were urged to do their best to
demonstrate the interpersonal skills listed on the
BOS
for the reasons cited earlier. They then met in groups
of four to six people to discuss the importance of
doing their best to demonstrate the
BOS items.
Consistent with previous goal-setting studies (see
Locke & Latham, 1990), these people did not complete
questionnaires concerning a goal as they were
not in a goal-setting condition.
Training in
VSG. Three weekly 90-minute VSG
training sessions were provided. In the first session,
immediately following the goal-setting intervention,
the participants (n = 60) discussed ways that their
positive (i.e., functional) and negative (i.e., dysfunctional)
self-talk affected their behaviour in the
MBA
program. Each person identified three dysfunctional
self-statements concerning their performance. These
statements were the focus of training in
VSG.
Consistent with Meichenbaum (1975, 1977), people
were taught to change their dysfunctional self-statements
to functional self-talk. A three-step process
was used. Specifically, they were trained to become
aware of negative self-statements (e.g., “I just can’t
seem to motivate my team-mates”), then discover for
themselves specific ways they could improve the situation,
and finally to develop positive self-statements
to guide their actions (e.g., “I have already learned to
give positive feedback…I can use positive feedback
to energize my team-mates.”).
Consistent with Meichenbaum and Goodman
(1971), each statement was initially modeled by the
trainer. Then each trainee repeated the statement
overtly. Finally, each trainee repeated the statement
covertly. The session ended with the trainees receiving
logbooks to record their self-statements concerning
their performance over the coming weeks.
The second session contained three components.
First, the trainer reviewed the
VSG technique. Second,
trainees reviewed their logbooks and gave examples
of areas in which they had engaged in functional or
dysfunctional self-talk concerning their performance.
Third, for each dysfunctional statement, the trainees
applied the
VSG technique.
The third session replicated the second session. In
addition, trainees identified obstacles to successfully
using
VSG to increase their performance in the MBA
program. As a group, participants discussed these
obstacles and strategized ways to overcome them.
The session ended with questionnaires assessing
trainees’ reactions to the
VSG training.
In order to minimize demand effects, people in the
control condition (n = 66), who were not trained in
VSG
, took part in a simulation exercise where they
assumed the roles of members of a city council. This
council was asked to review several proposals concerning
the use of a building donated to the city. The
rationale given to the participants in this control condition
was that the simulation was designed to teach
them the importance of working effectively as individuals
in teams. Participants had 90 minutes to
reach group consensus concerning the proposal that
they would accept.
Data collection. At the end of the semester manipulation
checks were conducted. Prior to receiving their
grades, peers assessed one another’s interpersonal
skills anonymously. The dean’s office calculated the
GPA
for each individual.
Measures
VSG
manipulation checks. VSG manipulation checks
included trainee reactions and
VSG skills usage.
Reactions were assessed using a 10-item, 5-point
Likert-type scale (e.g., the extent to which
VSG participants:
a) would recommend this training to others,
b) were satisfied with the training, and c) found the
trainer helpful). In assessing
VSG usage, all participants
(
VSG and control) were asked the extent to
which they were aware of their self-statements, monitored
their self-statements, generated positive selfstatements,
and converted negative statements to
positive ones using a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 =
strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree).
Goal-setting manipulation checks. Consistent with
the recommendations of Locke and Latham (1990),
people in the goal condition completed questionnaires
assessing actual goal difficulty level (i.e.,
desired
BOS score), perceived goal difficulty level,
goal specificity, and goal commitment. The latter
three measures were assessed using a 5-point Likerttype
scale where 1 = strongly disagree and 5 =
strongly agree. Perceived goal difficulty and specificity
were assessed at the end of the study using questions
adapted from Winters and Latham (1996).
Specifically, two goal difficulty questions asked if
participants saw their goal as difficult (Question 1) or
easy (Question 2, reverse scored). Goal specificity
was measured using three questions, namely, the
extent to which participants believed that their goal
Verbal Self-Guidance 7
was specific (Question 1), vague (Question 2, reverse
scored), or uncertain (Question 3, reverse scored). A
person’s goal was operationalized in terms of
BOS
score; goal commitment was assessed using the fiveitem
goal commitment scale developed by Klein,
Wesson, Hollenbeck, Wright, and DeShon (2001).
Performance. The behavioural measure of an individual’s
interpersonal skills was anonymously
assessed by peers. Specifically, peers rated the frequency
with which they observed a team member
performing each behaviour using a 5-point scale (0 =
almost never and 4 = almost always). These peers
worked with the person throughout the entire semester.
Thus, peers were arguably the most important
source of information regarding a person’s behaviour.
The outcome measure of a person’s performance
(i.e.,
GPA) was measured by faculty who were
neither aware of the purpose of the study, nor the
experimental condition to which a person had been
randomly assigned. The composite criterion, performance
effectiveness, was created by summing each
person’s z-score for
GPA and BOS.
Results
VSG
Manipulation Checks
Responses indicated that the
VSG participants
were satisfied with the training (
M = 39.46; SD = 5.02;
maximum score = 50, α = .79). Moreover, they reported
greater use of
VSG skills (M = 19.28; SD = 3.51;
maximum score = 25; α = .91) than those in the control
(
M = 15.43; SD = 4.91) condition, F(1, 108) = 22.00,
p < .001.
Goal-Setting Manipulation Checks
Goal-setting manipulation checks revealed that
the mean goal set by goal-setting participants was am
47.07 (
SD = 4.08; maximum score = 56), that these
participants were highly committed to their goals (
M
= 21.12;
SD = 2.94; α = .71; maximum score = 25), and
that they perceived their goals to be both specific (
M
= 11.42;
SD = 2.30; α = .66; maximum score = 15) and
difficult (
M = 7.98; SD = 1.44; α = .65; maximum score
= 10).
ANOVA confirmed that there were no significant
differences in these four measures between the
goal-setting participants in the
VSG condition and
those in the control condition,
F(1, 58) = 1.50, p > .05;
F
(1, 57) = .15, p >. 05; F(1, 48) = .03, p > .05; F(1, 48) =
.01, p > .05, respectively.
Performance
GPA
. GPA was available for 121 participants; 5 had
left the
MBA program. GPA ranged from 2.68 to 3.89
(
M = 3.44; SD = .21; maximum score = 4). Consistent
with the first hypothesis, a 2 x 2
ANOVA revealed a
main effect for
VSG on GPA, F(1, 117) = 6.60, p < .01. In
addition, there was an interaction effect between
VSG
and goal setting,
F(1, 117) = 4.02, p < .05. Thus,
Hypotheses 1 and 2 were supported. No main effect
was found for goal setting,
F(1, 117) = .01, p > .05. The
means and standard deviations are shown in Table 2.
The interaction effect is presented in Figure 1.
Bobko (1986) argued that a spurious main effect
can result in a 2 x 2
ANOVA when an ordinal interaction
is present. Given our hypotheses that the participants
in the
VSG/goal-setting condition would have
the highest performance level, we tested the interaction
using Bobko’s ordinal interaction technique. This
technique involves two planned contrasts: 1) testing
the equality of the means of the three noninteraction
conditions (i.e.,
VSG/DYB; Control/ Goal Setting;
Control/
DYB) using a one-way analysis; and 2) comparing
the means of the interaction (i.e.,
VSG/goal
setting) versus the average of the remaining three
conditions using a planned t-test. Contrast one was
not significant,
F(2, 90) = 2.01, p > .05); thus, there was
no significant difference among the means of the
three conditions. Contrast two was significant, t(119)
= 2.51, p < .05. Thus, there was an ordinal interaction
with the
VSG/goal-setting condition having the highest
level of
GPA relative to the other three study conditions.
Interpersonal skills. For the behavioural measure of
performance,
BOS scores were obtained for 117 participants
(response rate = 92.86%).The level of agreement
among raters was calculated using the average
interrater agreement statistic, r
wg, (James, Demaree,
& Wolf, 1993). The r
wg, for the 11 behavioural items
ranged from .71 to .84; the average level of agreement
across the 11 items was .77. The median number of
Verbal Self-Guidance 9
peer ratings per person was four. Cronbach’s alpha
coefficient for the 11-item
BOS was .90. The grand
mean of the 11-item
BOS was 36.26 (SD = 4.17; maximum
score = 44).
The correlation between GPA and interpersonal
skills was significant (r = .43, p < .01). Consistent with
Hypothesis 2, a 2 x 2
ANOVA revealed a significant
interaction effect,
F(1, 113) = 3.97, p < .05. There were
no main effects for either
VSG or goal setting on interpersonal
skills,
F(1, 113) = .01, p >.05; F(1, 113) = .22, p
> .05, respectively. The means and standard deviations
of this
ANOVA are shown in Table 2; the interaction
effect is presented in Figure 2. Again, Bobko’s
(1986) ordinal interaction analysis was conducted.
For contrast one,
ANOVA revealed no significant differences
among the three conditions of
VSG only, goal
setting only, and Control/
DYB, F(2, 88) = .99, p > .05.
In addition, there was no significant difference
between the means of the
VSG/goal setting and the
mean of the other three conditions, t(115) = 1.41, p =
.16.
Composite criterion. A 2 x 2
ANOVA of the composite
criterion of performance effectiveness revealed a
main effect for
VSG, F(1, 111) = 4.11, p < .05. There was
also an interaction effect between
VSG and goal setting
,
F(1, 111 ) = 5.12, p < .05. Thus, Hypotheses 1 and
2 were supported. No main effect was found for goal
setting,
F(1, 111) = .28, p > .05. The means and standard
deviations are shown in Table 2. The interaction
effect is presented in Figure 3.
Again, Bobko’s (1986) ordinal interaction analysis
was conducted to see if the main effect for
VSG was
spurious. For contrast one,
ANOVA revealed no significant
differences among the three conditions of
VSG
only, goal setting only, and Control/DYB, F(2, 87)
= .88, p > .05. For contrast two, there was a significant
difference between the mean of the
VSG/goal-setting
condition versus the mean of the other three conditions,
t(113) = 2.66, p < .01. Thus, there was evidence
of an ordinal interaction for the composite criterion.
Discussion
The theoretical and practical significance of this
experiment is three-fold. First, of significance to both
goal-setting theory and training in
VSG is the interaction
effect found for
VSG and goal setting on performance.
The lack of a main effect for goal setting alone
is consistent with previous findings that when people
are in a learning mode, setting a specific, difficult
performance goal does not improve performance
(Kanfer & Ackerman, 1989; Seijts & Latham, 2001). In
the present study, the task of mastering the MBA curriculum
and the requisite interpersonal skills,
required more than motivation in terms of choice,
effort, and persistence; it required learning interpersonal
skills. This finding supports Locke’s (2000) conclusion
that the highest level of performance occurs
when participants have high task knowledge and
high motivation. In the present study, performance
was highest in the condition where participants were
trained to use
VSG and to set a high performance goal
that motivated them to apply their newly acquired
knowledge and skill.
Second, this study, when examined in conjunction
with the four previous industrial-organizational psychology
studies involving
VSG, suggests three tentative
answers to the question: “When is
VSG training
effective?” First,
VSG appears to be a skill that
requires multiple training sessions of several hours
duration. In the present study, and that of both
Brown (2003) and Millman and Latham (2001),
trainees took part in two or more training sessions
where the total training program lasted at least 100
minutes. In contrast, a single, short training session
of less than 40 minutes failed to result in a significant
main effect (Waung, 1995). Second,
VSG appears to be
effective when people lack the knowledge and skill to
perform the task at hand. When participants have
already mastered a task, and thus have the requisite
knowledge and skill,
VSG does not appear to increase
performance. This argument is supported by the
findings of Neck and Manz (1996), where
VSG did
not increase the performance of employees. In that
study, the sample consisted of experienced employees
who had already gained the knowledge and skill
needed to effectively perform their jobs. Third,
Bobko’s (1986) ordinal interaction analysis suggests
that the main effect for
VSG on GPA as well as the
composite criterion was spurious.
VSG appears to be
most effective when combined with an explicitly set
behavioural goal.
An unexpected finding was that there was not an
ordinal interaction when the dependent variable was
interpersonal skills, despite the moderately high correlation
between the two performance measures (i.e.,
GPA
and BOS score). As previously mentioned,
behavioural measures are prone to observation errors
such as halo and leniency. With a mean of 36.26 on
the 11-item
BOS, most participants received a rating
of greater than three for each of the 11 items on the
BOS
scale, where the maximum score for each item
was four. Restriction in range is likely responsible for
the present finding.
Third, the results of this study may prove useful
for increasing the persuasiveness of
MBA directors’
messages regarding the importance of teamwork
skills for people who enter the
MBA program with little
appreciation for interpersonal effectiveness.
Interpersonal skills are important for one’s
GPA. The
present study shows that only five hours of training
(i.e., 270 minutes for
VSG plus 30 minutes for goal setting)
can provide
MBA students the self-regulation
skills necessary for increasing their overall performance.
Training in
VSG is a practical intervention that
can be included in existing organizational behaviour
courses (e.g., courses on management skills).
Potential limitations of this experiment include the
sample and the context. In this study, participants
were adults enrolled in an educational institution.
While the participants were, on average, 29 years old
with 4 1/2 years of work experience, the extent to
which the findings from the present sample generalize
to older adults, particularly adults working in the
public and private sector organizations, remains to be
tested.
References
Bobko, P. (1986). A solution to some dilemmas when testing
hypotheses about ordinal interactions.
Journal of
Applied Psychology, 71,
323-326.
Brown, T. C. (2003). The effect of verbal self-guidance
training on collective efficacy and team performance.
Personnel Psychology, 56, 935-964.
de Janasz, S. C., Dowd, K. O., & Schneider, B. Z. (2002).
Interpersonal skills in organizations. Toronto, ON:
McGraw Hill.
Flanagan, J. C. (1954). The critical incident technique.
Psychological Bulletin, 51, 327-358.
Halpern, D. F. (2004). The development of adult cognition:
Understanding constancy and change in adult
learning. In D. Day, S. Zaccaro, & S. Halpin (Eds.),
Leader development for transforming organizations.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Jack, I. (2001, December 31). FP CEO Poll: MBAs good on
theory, lack people skills, CEOs say. National Post, pp.
FP1, FP2.
James, L. R., Demaree, R. G., & Wolf, G. (1993). RWG: An
assessment of within-group interrater agreement.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 306-309.
James, W. (1892). Psychology. London, UK: Macmillan.
Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (1989). Motivation and cog-
Verbal Self-Guidance 11
nitive abilities: An integrative/aptitude-treatment
interaction approach to skill acquisition.
Journal of
Applied Psychology, 74,
657-690.
Klein, H. J., Wesson, M. J., Hollenbeck, J. R., Wright, P. M.,
& DeShon, R. P. (2001). The assessment of goal commitment:
A measurement model meta-analysis.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
85, 32-55.
Latham, G. P. (1997).
Correlates of executive education program
revenue, repeat business and the satisfaction of client
organizations and individual participants.
Millington, NJ:
University Consortium for Executive Education (UNICON).
Latham, G. P., & Wexley, K. N. (1977). Behavioral observation
scales for performance appraisal purposes
.
Personnel Psychology, 30
, 255-268.
Latham, G. P., & Wexley, K. N. (1994).
Increasing productivity
through performance appraisal
(2nd ed.). Reading,
MA: Addison-Wexley.
Locke, E. (2000). Motivation, cognition, and action: An
analysis of studies of task goals and knowledge.
Applied Psychology: An International Review, 49, 408-429.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990).
A theory of goal setting
and task performance.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-
Hall.
Mason, J. C. (1992). Business schools: Striving to meet
customer demand. Management Review, 81, 10.
Meichenbaum, D. (1975). Enhancing creativity by modifying
what subjects say to themselves.
American
Educational Research Journal, 12
, 129-145.
Meichenbaum, D. H. (1971). Examination of model characteristics
in reducing avoidance behavior.
Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 17
, 298-307.
Meichenbaum, D. H. (1977).
Cognitive behavior modification.
An integrative approach
. New York: Plenum Press.
Meichenbaum, D., & Goodman, J. (1971). Training impulsive
children to talk to themselves: A means of developing
self-control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 77,
115-126.
Millman, Z., & Latham, G. P. (2001). Increasing reemployment
through training in verbal self-guidance. In M.
Erez, U. Kleinbeck, & H. Thierry (Eds.),
Work
Motivation in the Context of a Globalizing Economy
(pp.
87-97). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Neck, C. P., & Manz, C. C. (1996). Thought self-leadership:
The impact of mental strategies training on
employee cognition, behavior, and affect.
Journal of
Organizational Behavior, 17
, 445-467.
Olian, J., Caldwell, L., Frank, H., Griffin, A., Liverpool, P.,
Thomas, H., et al. (2002, April).
AACSB International:
Management Education Task Force
(Draft IV). St. Louis,
MO: The Association to Advanced Collegiate Schools
of Business International.
Pfeffer, J. (1998).
The human equation: Building profits by
putting people first
. Boston, MA: Harvard Business
School Press.
Schmidt, F. J., & Kaplan, L. (1971). Composite vs. multiple
criteria: A review and resolution of the controversy.
Personnel Psychology, 24, 419-434.
Schunk, D. H., & Rice, J. M. (1984). Strategy self-verbalization
during remedial listening comprehension
instruction. Journal of Experimental Education, 53, 49-54.
Schunk, D. H., & Rice, J. M. (1985). Verbalization of comprehension
strategies: Effects on children’s achievement
outcomes. Human Learning, 4, 1-10.
Seijts, G. H., & Latham, G. P. (2001). The effect of distal
learning, outcome, and proximal goals on a moderately
complex task. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 22,
291-302.
Sue-Chan, C., & Latham, G. P. (2004). The relative effectiveness
of external, peer, and self-coaches.
Applied
Psychology: An International Review, 52
, 260-278.
Taggar, S., & Brown, T. C. (2001). Problem-solving team
behaviors: Development and validation of BOS and a
hierarchical factor structure. Small Group Research, 32,
698-726.
Vygotsky, I. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Waung, M. (1995). The effects of self-regulatory coping
orientation on newcomer adjustment and job survival.
Personnel Psychology, 48, 633-651.
Whetten, D. A., & Cameron, K. S. (2002).
Developing management
skills
(5th ed.). Don Mills, ON: Addison-
Wesley.
Credit:ivythesis.typepad.com
0 comments:
Post a Comment
Click to see the code!
To insert emoticon you must added at least one space before the code.